Rough-and-tumble play, also called play fighting, is a form of play where participants compete with one another attempting to obtain certain advantages (such as biting or pushing the opponent onto the ground) but play in this way without the severity of genuine fighting (which rough-and-tumble play resembles). Rough-and-tumble play is one of the most common forms of play in both humans and non-human animals.[1]
It has been pointed out that despite its apparent aggressiveness, rough-and-tumble play is helpful for encouraging cooperative behavior and cultivation of social skills. For rough-and-tumble play to remain "play" (instead of spiraling into a real fight), there has to be cooperation (e.g., with participants agreeing to not actually exert forces in pretend punches). Sometimes, one participant may push or hit harder than expected, and then the other participants will have to decide whether it was an unintended mistake or a malicious transgression. Thus, rough-and-tumble play involves considerable social reasoning and judgment.[2][3]
Sexual Dimorphism
This form of play exhibits notable sexual dimorphism in many mammalian species, including humans. Males typically engage in this type of play more frequently and intensively than females, a pattern observed across diverse taxa ranging from rodents to primates. This dimorphism is thought to result from the influence of prenatal and early postnatal hormones, particularly androgens like testosterone, which shape the neural circuits governing social and play behavior. Researchers propose that these sex differences may have evolutionary significance, contributing to the development of motor skills, dominance behaviors, and social strategies that were advantageous for reproductive success in ancestral environments.[4][5][6][7]
Mammals
Carnivora
Male carnivores tend to have higher rates of rough-and-tumble play compared to females. This is potentially due to the fact that males are typically larger, due to polygamous or promiscuous mating systems, as well as energy requirements imposed on females by feeding young.[4]
Primates are known for their relatively long juvenile development periods, which are often associated with juvenile play. Long juvenile periods, small litter sizes, long intervals between births, extended lifespans and high investment in offsprings mean that primates are often an ideal model for looking at play behaviour.[4]
In the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta), female infants are found to engage in social play more frequently than male conspecifics, although there are some conflicting reports.[24][25][26][27]
Geladas (Theropithecus gelada) show no sexed differences in rough-and-tumble play, although some reports suggest that in the wild, males play slightly more often than females.[50][51]
Vervets (Cercopithecus aethiops) have juvenile males that engage more frequently and more aggressively in rough-and-tumble play. It is suggested however that these patterns are influenced heavily by age in this species, and that females engage in more social play later in their development (48 to 61 months).[52][53][54]
In chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), males are known to engage more frequently in play than females. Age, setting, components of play and age group are all known factors that affect how long and how often immature chimpanzees engage in rough-and-tumble play.[63][64]
Rodents
Rough-and-tumble play has been observed in captive prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and wild capybara (Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris), irrespective of sex. In wild yellow-bellied marmosets (Marmota flaviventris), male yearlings participate in bouts more frequently than female conspecifics.[65][66][67]
In Columbian ground squirrels (Spermophilus columbianus), reports vary, with some suggesting that males engage and participate in rough-and-tumble play more frequently, while others suggest there is no significant observed sex bias. Belding's ground squirrels (Urocitellus beldingi) also engage in juvenile rough-and-tumble play.[68][69][70]
Results from captive golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) suggest that males engage more frequently in bouts and longer than females. This pattern is also observed in the juvenile rough-and-tumble play behavior of captive hooded rats (Rattus norvegicus).[71][72]
Cetaceans (Whales and dolphins)
Low levels of social play have been recorded in both sexes of bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) calves.[73][74]
Rough-and-tumble play has been observed in belugas (Delphinapterus leucas), in both juveniles and adults.[75]
Chiroptera (Bats)
Rough-and-tumble play has been observed in the common vampire bat (Desmodus rotundus), with a male bias towards initiation.[76]
Dasyuromorphia (Carnivorous marsupials)
In captive kowaris (Dasyuroides byrnie), rough-and-tumble play has been observed in both sexes of juveniles.[77] Similarly, juvenile rough-and-tumble play has been reported in both sexes of spotted-tail quolls (Dasyurus maculatus).[78]
Diprotodontia (Marsupials)
In captive, red-necked wallabies (Macropus rufogriseus banksianus) rough-and-tumble play is reported. This play behaviour is more common in males, with juvenile females rarely observed engaging.[79]
Playfighting has been reported in the red kangaroo (Macropus rufus) between mother and offspring.[80]
Perissodactyla (Odd-toed ungulates)
Multiple studies have investigated rough-and-tumble play in horses (Equus caballus). One study reports that male Jeju pony foals are more likely to engage in this kind of play after grooming compared to female conspecifics. Another study reports that juvenile male Icelandic horses engage in rough-and-tumble play more frequently than juvenile females. This pattern has also been observed in Welsh ponies, Camargue's and feral horses.[81][82][83][84][85]
Artiodactyla (Even-toed ungulates)
Play-fighting has been reported in adult and juvenile male goitered gazelles (Gazella subgutturosa).[86] Similarly, it is also reported in juvenile pronghorns (Antilocapra americana) and guanacos (Lama guanicoe).[87][88]
Proboscidea (Elephants)
Rough-and-tumble play is observed in both African (Loxodonta africana) and Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) calves. It has been noted that there is an increase in male bias in captive animals compared to wild.[89][90]
^Suomi, S.J. (2005). "Genetic and environmental factors influencing the expression of impulsive aggression and serotonergic functioning in rhesus monkeys". In Tremblay, Richard; Archer, John; Hartup, William W. (eds.). Developmental origins of aggression. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 63–82. ISBN978-1-59385-110-1. OCLC68667321.
^Olioff, Mark; Stewart, Jane (February 1978). "Sex differences in the play behavior of prepubescent rats". Physiology & Behavior. 20 (2): 113–115. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(78)90060-4. PMID662934.
^Renouf, D.; Lawson, J. W. (1987). "Quantitative aspects of harbour seal (Phoca vitulina) play". Journal of Zoology. 212 (2): 267–273. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1987.tb05989.x.
^Ward, Camille; Bauer, Erika B.; Smuts, Barbara B. (October 2008). "Partner preferences and asymmetries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates". Animal Behaviour. 76 (4): 1187–1199. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004.
^Koscinczuk, P.; Alabarcez, M. N.; Cainzos, R. P. (2015). "Social play traits and environmental exploration in beagle and fox terriers' puppies". Revista Veterinaria. 26 (1): 33–37. doi:10.30972/vet.261245.
^Pal, Sunil Kumar (September 2010). "Play behaviour during early ontogeny in free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris)". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 126 (3–4): 140–153. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2010.06.005.
^Vincent, Louis E.; Bekoff, Marc (February 1978). "Quantitative analyses of the ontogeny of predatory behaviour in coyotes, Canis latrans". Animal Behaviour. 26: 225–231. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(78)90022-2.
^Biben, Maxeen (August 1983). "Comparative ontogeny of social behaviour in three South American canids, the maned wolf, crab-eating fox and bush dog: Implications for sociality". Animal Behaviour. 31 (3): 814–826. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80238-3.
^Alekseeva, G. S.; Antonevich, A. L.; Erofeeva, M. N.; Naidenko, S. V. (July 2014). "Social play in the development of sibling relations in Eurasian lynx (Lynx lynx)". Biology Bulletin. 41 (4): 364–371. Bibcode:2014BioBu..41..364A. doi:10.1134/S1062359014040025.
^Caro, T.M. (1981). "Predatory Behaviour and Social Play in Kittens". Behaviour. 76 (1–2): 1–24. doi:10.1163/156853981X00013.
^Sharpe, L.L.; Cherry, M.I. (November 2003). "Social play does not reduce aggression in wild meerkats". Animal Behaviour. 66 (5): 989–997. doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2275.
^Gould, Lisa (August 1990). "The social development of free-ranging infantLemur catta at Berenty reserve, Madagascar". International Journal of Primatology. 11 (4): 297–318. doi:10.1007/BF02193002.
^Meredith, Stephanie L. (14 February 2018). "Sex-Typed Social Development in Lemur catta". Folia Primatologica. 89 (3–4): 224–239. doi:10.1159/000487433. PMID29734175.
^McDaniel PS (1994) The social behavior and ecology of the black-handed spider monkey (Ateles geofroyi). Dissertation, Saint Louis University
^Rizzo KA (2004) The socioecology of Alouatta pigra at the Calakmul Archeological Zone and Biosphere Reserve, Mexico. Dissertation, University of Illinois.
^Zucker, Evan L.; Clarke, Margaret R. (1992). "Developmental and Comparative Aspects of Social Play of Mantled Howling Monkeys in Costa Rica". Behaviour. 123 (1–2): 144–171. doi:10.1163/156853992X00165.
^Box, Hilary O. (1975-06-01). "Quantitative studies of behaviour within captive groups of marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus)". Primates. 16 (2): 155–174. doi:10.1007/BF02381414.
^Vogt, Jerry L. (1978-04-01). "The social behavior of a marmoset (Saguinus fuscicollis) group II: Behavior patterns and social interaction". Primates. 19 (2): 287–300. doi:10.1007/BF02382798.
^Welker, Christian; Becker, Petra; Höhmann, Hella; Schäfer-Witt, Cornelia (14 February 1990). "Social Relations in Groups of the Black-Capped Capuchin (Cebus apella) in Captivity". Folia Primatologica. 54 (1–2): 16–33. doi:10.1159/000156423. PMID2323687.
^Eaton, G. Gray; Johnson, Deanne F.; Glick, Barbara B.; Worlein, Julie M. (July 1985). "Development in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): Sexually dimorphic behavior during the first year of life". Primates. 26 (3): 238–247. doi:10.1007/BF02382400.
^Eaton, G. Gray; Johnson, Deanne F.; Glick, Barbara B.; Worlein, Julie M. (April 1986). "Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) social development: Sex differences in Juvenile behavior". Primates. 27 (2): 141–150. doi:10.1007/BF02382594.
^Glick, Barbara Beckerman; Eaton, G. Gray; Johnson, Deanne F.; Worlein, Julie M. (October 1986). "Development of partner preferences in Japanese Macaques (Macaca fuscata): Effects of gender and kinship during the second year of life". International Journal of Primatology. 7 (5): 467–479. doi:10.1007/BF02693658.
^Petit, O.; Bertrand, F.; Thierry, B. (May 2008). "Social play in crested and japanese macaques: Testing the covariation hypothesis". Developmental Psychobiology. 50 (4): 399–407. doi:10.1002/dev.20305. ISSN0012-1630. PMID18393281. S2CID45340145.
^Young, George H.; Hankins, Rebecca J. (January 1979). "Infant behaviors in mother-reared and harem-reared baboons (Papio cynocephalus)". Primates. 20 (1): 87–93. doi:10.1007/BF02373830.
^Cheney, Dorothy L. (November 1978). "The play partners of immature baboons". Animal Behaviour. 26: 1038–1050. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(78)90093-3.
^Chalmers, N.R. (May 1980). "The ontogeny of play in feral olive baboons (Papio anubis)". Animal Behaviour. 28 (2): 570–585. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80066-2.
^Owens, N.W. (November 1975). "A comparison of aggressive play and aggression in free-living baboons, Papio anubis". Animal Behaviour. 23: 757–765. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(75)90103-7. PMID1239227.
^Mancini, Giada; Palagi, Elisabetta (November 2009). "Play and social dynamics in a captive herd of gelada baboons (Theropithecus gelada)". Behavioural Processes. 82 (3): 286–292. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2009.07.007. PMID19635533.
^Raleigh, Michael J.; Flannery, Jeffrey W.; Ervin, Frank R. (August 1979). "Sex differences in behavior among juvenile vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus)". Behavioral and Neural Biology. 26 (4): 455–465. doi:10.1016/S0163-1047(79)91482-1.
^Fedigan, Linda (December 1972). "Social and solitary play in a colony of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops)". Primates. 13 (4): 347–364. doi:10.1007/BF01793655. PMID27520640.
^Govindarajulu, P.; Hunte, W.; Vermeer, L. A.; Horrocks, J. A. (October 1993). "The ontogeny of social play in a feral troop of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus): The function of early play". International Journal of Primatology. 14 (5): 701–719. doi:10.1007/BF02192187.
^Macleod, MC. (2000). The reproductive strategies of Samango monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis erythrarchus). Dissertation, University of Surrey.
^Wolfheim, Jaclyn H. (1977). "Sex Differences in Behavior in a Group of Captive Juvenile Talapoin Monkeys (Miopithecus talapoin)". Behaviour. 63 (1/2): 110–128. doi:10.1163/156853977X00199. JSTOR4533849.
^Rowell, T. E.; Chism, Janice (February 1986). "The ontogeny of sex differences in the behavior of patas monkeys". International Journal of Primatology. 7 (1): 83–107. doi:10.1007/BF02692310.
^ abWorch, Eric A. (14 February 2010). "Play Behavior of Red Colobus Monkeys in Kibale National Park, Uganda". Folia Primatologica. 81 (3): 163–176. doi:10.1159/000317744. PMID20881405.
^Förster, Steffen; Cords, Marina (2005). "Socialization of Infant Blue Monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni): Allomaternal Interactions and Sex Differences". Behaviour. 142 (7): 869–896. doi:10.1163/1568539055010138. JSTOR4536276.
^Bernstein, Irwin S. (1976). "Activity Patterns in a Sooty Mangabey Group". Folia Primatologica. 26 (3): 185–206. doi:10.1159/000155750. PMID824181.
^Armitage, Kenneth B. (February 1974). "Male behaviour and territoriality in the Yellow-bellied marmot". Journal of Zoology. 172 (2): 233–265. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1974.tb04104.x.
^Waterman, Jane M. (May 1986). "Behaviour and use of space by juvenile Columbian ground squirrels ( Spermophilus columbianus )". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 64 (5): 1121–1127. Bibcode:1986CaJZ...64.1121W. doi:10.1139/z86-168.
^Marks, Karen A; Vizconde, Daniel L; Gibson, Emma S; Rodriguez, Jennifer R; Nunes, Scott (August 2017). "Play behavior and responses to novel situations in juvenile ground squirrels". Journal of Mammalogy. 98 (4): 1202–1210. doi:10.1093/jmammal/gyx049.
^Goldman, L.; Swanson, Heidi H. (March 1975). "Developmental changes in pre-adult behavior in confined colonies of golden hamsters". Developmental Psychobiology. 8 (2): 137–150. doi:10.1002/dev.420080206. PMID1241532.
^Meaney, Michael J.; Stewart, Jane (February 1981). "A descriptive study of social development in the rat (Rattus norvegicus)". Animal Behaviour. 29 (1): 34–45. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80149-2.
^Soriano Jimenez, Ana Isabel; Drago, Massimiliano; Vinyoles, Dolors; Maté, Carmen (3 July 2021). "Play Behavior in Two Captive Bottlenose Dolphin Calves ( Tursiops Truncatus ): Ethogram, Ontogeny, and Individual Differences". Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science. 24 (3): 292–320. doi:10.1080/10888705.2021.1902811. hdl:2445/183727. PMID33890533.
^Ham, Jackson Robert (2021). Social and Non-Social Play in Beluga Whales (Delphinapterus leucas) (Thesis). ProQuest2623849898.[page needed]
^Park, Shi-Ryong (1990). "Observation on the Behavioral Development of the Common Vampire Bat Desmodus rotundus—Play Behaviros". Journal of the Mammalogical Society of Japan. 15 (1): 25–32. doi:10.11238/jmammsocjapan1987.15.25.
^Meißner, Karin; Gansloßer, Udo (January 1985). "Development of young in the kowari Dasyuroides byrnei spencer, 1896". Zoo Biology. 4 (4): 351–359. doi:10.1002/zoo.1430040405.
^Körtner, Gerhard; Claridge, Andrew; Ballard, Guy (2019). "Denning behaviour of female spotted-tailed quolls during the breeding season". Australian Journal of Zoology. 67 (3): 145–152. doi:10.1071/ZO20012.
^Watson, Duncan M.; Croft, David B. (1993). "Playfighting in Captive Red-Necked Wallabies, Macropus rufogriseus banksianus". Behaviour. 126 (3/4): 219–245. doi:10.1163/156853993X00128. JSTOR4535135.
^Wilhelm, P.; Ganslosser, U. (1989). "Sequential orgaqisation of social behaviour in captive adult and juvenile Macropus rufus (Marsupialia: Macropodidae)". Australian Mammalogy. 12 (1): 5–13. doi:10.1071/am89001.
^Rho, Jeong Rae; Srygley, Robert Baxter; Choe, Jae Chun (2007-08-01). "Sex Preferences in Jeju Pony Foals (Equus caballus) for Mutual Grooming and Play-Fighting Behaviors". Zoological Science. 24 (8): 769–773. doi:10.2108/zsj.24.769. PMID18217482.
^Sigurjónsdóttir, Hrefna; Snorrason, Sigurdur; van Dierendonck, Machteld; Thórhallsdóttir, Anna (2003). "Social relationships in a group of horses without a mature stallion". Behaviour. 140 (6): 783–804. doi:10.1163/156853903322370670.
^Crowell-Davis, S.L.; Houpt, K.A.; Kane, L. (August 1987). "Play development in Welsh pony (Equus caballus) foals". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 18 (2): 119–131. doi:10.1016/0168-1591(87)90186-9.
^Wells, Susan M.; Goldschmidt-Rothschild, Bettina (26 April 2010). "Social Behaviour and Relationships in a Herd of Camargue Horses". Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 49 (4): 363–380. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1979.tb00299.x.
^Cameron, Elissa Z.; Linklater, Wayne L.; Stafford, Kevin J.; Minot, Edward O. (November 2008). "Maternal investment results in better foal condition through increased play behaviour in horses". Animal Behaviour. 76 (5): 1511–1518. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.009.
^Miller, Michelle N.; Byers, John A. (1998), Byers, John A.; Bekoff, Marc (eds.), "Sparring as play in young pronghorn males", Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative and Ecological Perspectives, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 141–160, ISBN978-0-521-58656-6, retrieved 2025-01-16
^Wilson, Paul; Franklin, William L. (1985). "Male Group Dynamics and Inter-male Aggression of Guanacos in Southern Chile)". Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 69 (4): 305–328. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1985.tb00155.x.
^Webber, Catherine Elizabeth (15 December 2017). A Comparison of Behavioural Development of Elephant Calves in Captivity and in the Wild: Implications for Welfare (Thesis). hdl:1893/27503.